Mostrando postagens com marcador NTSB. Mostrar todas as postagens
Mostrando postagens com marcador NTSB. Mostrar todas as postagens

domingo, 1 de janeiro de 2023

WAS IT A FLARE ACCIDENT OR LANDING ACCIDENT?

 


INVESTIGATION MUST REPORT IF THE ACCIDENT WAS A LANDING ACCIDENT OR A FLARE ACCIDENT


Mastering the Landing Flare/Round out

Pilot Perception

Analysis of 6,676 aircraft accident reports published by the National Transportation Safety Board

134 pilots with varying experience levels

Sources:

  •           THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF AVIATION PSYCHOLOGY, 12(2), 137–152
  •      Oklahoma State University
  •      Department of Psychology
  •      Laboratory of Comparative and  Behavioral Biology

           Danny Benbassat and Charles I. Abramson

  •  Razia Rashid - founder of Psychology To Safety
  •  ATSB RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS REPORT
  • Aviation Safety Research Grant – B2005/0119

Aerial perspective is a monocular cue which is used for depth perception [the atmosphere causes distant objects to look hazy or blurry], which is used to judge how far away objects are. Monocular cues are named because they can occur only using one eye (as opposed to binocular cues which only occur with the use of both eyes).

Critical perceived time to contact (TTC)



Time remaining until the aircraft’s wheels make contact with the runway if no further action is taken (Mulder et al 2000).

TTC must be estimated directly, without first estimating speed and distance, based on the following ratio (known as tau):

TTC ≈ θ/(dθ/dt)

Where θ is the visual angle between the aimpoint and any other point on the ground plane at time one (see Figure above); and dθ/dt is the rate of change of this angle over time (Hoyle F 1957; Kaiser & Mowafy 1993; Lee, DN 1976).

Tau is a monocular cue to time-to-contact. During a landing, tau can be defined as the ratio of the angular distance between any two points on the ground (which happen to lie along the aiming line) divided by how fast this angular distance is increasing. Several other versions of tau have been proposed (see Regan & Gray 2000).

In more recent aviation research, Mulder and colleagues (2000) found mixed support for the proposals that flare timing is based on perceived runway angle (ψ) and perceived TTC (based on tau).

Study

The effects of pilot’s critical flare operation on long and hard landing events based on real flight Quick Access Recorder (QAR) data.

Flare accident rates

Causes for improper flares

The flare is the transition from a controlled descent to actual contact with the landing surface (Federal Aviation Administration [FAA], 1999; Grosz et al., 1995) and is also known as the flare out, round out, or level off (Jeppesen, 1985).

The task of determining the aircraft altitude above ground is crucial to a successful flare (Green, Muir, James, Gradwell, & Green, 1996) and is accomplished by the use of vision more than any other sense (FAA, 1999; Jeppesen, 1985; Menon, 1996; Nagel, 1988; Thom, 1992).

Depth Perception

Depth perception deals with the ability to see the environment in three dimensions and estimate distances of objects from us and from each other.

Aerial Perspective, objects at larger distances from us are affected by natural scattering of light and form less of a contrast with their background; making it harder to gauge a distance between the two and us.



Monocular cues operate when a person is looking with only one eye

 Pilots rely on monocular depth perception cues rather than binocular depth perception cues (Benson, 1999; Bond, Bryan, Rigney, & Warren, 1962; Langewiesche, 1972; Nagel, 1988).

Monocular depth perception is learned or dependent on experience (Benson, 1999; Bramson, 1982; Langewiesche, 1972; Love, 1995; Marieb, 1995; Tredici, 1996).

-          It is used by artists to induce depth in their two dimensional paintings. Thus, they are also called as the pictorial cues.

The monocular cues that help us in judging the depth and distance in two-dimensional surfaces:

·         Linear perspective: Imagine you are standing between the rail tracks and looking in the distance. You would see that the rail track become smaller and smaller, until there is a point where they meet each other. Linear perspective reflects this phenomena. It says that the distant between two objects far away appear to be smaller than what the distance actually is.

·         Interposition or overlapping:  This monocular cue occurs when one object covers the other. The object that is completely visible seems to be nearer and the object that is partially visible seems to be farther away.

·         Clearness: The more clear the object the nearer it seems. For example, on a hazy day a distant mountain would appear far away than on a clear day, because haze blurs the fine details. Therefore, if we see details of an object we perceive it closer.

·         Relative height: We perceive large objects to be closer to us and smaller objects to be farther away.

·         Motion parallax/ Movement: This cue occurs when the objects are in motion. The distant object appears to move slowly than the objects that are close.



Binocular cues operate when both our eyes are working together

Binocular depth perception is innate or acquired very early in life (Fox, Aslin, Shea, & Dumais, 1980; Kalat, 1998; Reading, 1983; Reinecke & Simons, 1974).


The cues that are provided by both the eyes working together:

·         Retinal or binocular disparity: Humans have two eyes which are horizontally separated by a distance of 6.5 centimeters. Because of this distance between two eyes the images formed on retina of a same object is slightly different. This difference in the images of two eyes is retinal disparity.

·         Convergence: when we see a closer object our eyes turn inward or converge, so that the image is formed on the fovea. Some muscles in our eyes send signal to the brain regarding the degree of convergence and our brain interprets it as a cue for depth perception. The more your eyes turn inward or converge the nearer the objects appear in the space.

·         Accommodation: The process by which ciliary muscles change the focal length of the eyes so that the image is clearly formed on the retina is called the accommodation of the eye. The accommodation varies for near and distant objects and also for objects moving away or towards the eye.

Failure to accurately determine aircraft altitude may result in flaring the aircraft too high (Gleim, 1998; King, 1999; Quinlan, 1999) or too low above the runway (Christy, 1991; Kershner, 1981; Love, 1995).

Improper flares also increase brake, nosewheel tire, and nosewheel shimmy dampener wear (on Cessnas; Christy, 1991; Jorgensen & Schley, 1990).

Improper flares may affect pilot self-esteem and selfefficacy.

The flare maneuver was defined as the ability to determine 10 to 20 ft from the ground and initiate the leveloff.

Three groups of pilots (novice, intermediate, and expert) were surveyed with purposive sampling.

Participants were 134 pilots (novice = 55, intermediate = 45, expert = 34) from three Part 141-approved flight schools in the state of Oklahoma.

Pilot perceptions were assessed with a 21-item questionnaire.

Pilots were asked to rate the flare maneuver and nine other randomly selected standard flight maneuvers for the level of difficulty on a scale ranging from 1 (extremely easy) to 7 (extremely difficult) under optimal conditions (i.e., no wind, 10 miles visibility).

After rating the 10 items, pilots turned the page and learned that the study was specific to the landing flare.

In Item 11

Pilots were provided with the number of total annual U.S. landing accidents and were asked to estimate the number of annual flare accident frequencies.

In Item 12

Pilots were asked to indicate how confident they were in their estimates of annual flare accident frequencies on a scale ranging from 1 (low confident) to 7 (high confidence).

The next items were not only specific to the landing flare but also to their ability to determine when to initiate the flare, that is, estimate 10 to 20 ft from the ground.

In Item 13

Pilots imagined that they were transitioning from descent attitude to flare attitude and indicated how confident they were that their aircraft was 10 to 20 ft from the ground on a scale ranging from 1 (low confidence) to 7 (high confidence).

In Item 14

Pilots recalled their first solo flare attempts and rated factors that assisted them in determining the aircraft altitude before initiating the flare (CFI instruction, instrument readings, practice, pilot manual, ground-school training, other) on a 7-point-scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (to great extent).

In Item 15

After a reminder that pilots flare the aircraft 10 to 20 ft from the ground, ascertained how pilots rated the task of judging altitude when initiating the flare on a scale ranging from 1 (very easy) to 7 (very difficult).

In Item 16

Pilots imagined that they were on approach for landing and were asked to choose how they determine when to initiate the flare. how did they know they were 10 to 20 ft from the ground (instrument readings, gut reaction, I don’t, sense of sight, sense of balance, other).

In Item 17

Pilots were asked to indicate if there was a need for improved flare-training methods, on a scale ranging from 1 (definitely yes) to 7 (definitely no).

In Item 18

To what factors (pattern practice, natural ability, sheer luck, aviation books, my instructor, other) did they attribute their current successful landing flares, on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (to great extent).

Items 19 and 20 (reiterated Items 16 and 18)

Required pilots to elaborate and explain their responses. Pilots were instructed to think carefully before they answered and be as specific as possible.

In Item 21

Pilots were asked to indicate what type of visual information assisted them in determining when to initiate the flare.

CONCLUSION

 Overall, 6,676 accident reports produced by the NTSB were analyzed for flare accident rates.



Flare accidents

Proper flares depend on monocular cues, and monocular cues depend on experience (Hawkins, 1993; Rinalducci, Patterson, Forren, & Andes, 1985).

On average, the flare only lasts approximately 6 sec and a pilot with a total time of 5,000 hr only has approximately 8 hr of flare time (King, 1998).


In more recent aviation research, Mulder and colleagues (2000) found mixed support for the proposals that flare timing is based on perceived runway angle (ψ) and perceived TTC (based on tau).

Critical perceived runway angle (ψ)

Pilots could initiate the landing flare when the visual angle (ψ) formed between the left and right edges of the runway at the aiming line reaches a critical value (Mulder et al, 2000).

In fact, pilots use different cues or combinations of monocular cues. For example, overall, the horizon and end of runway, shape of runway or runway markings, and familiar objects were the most frequent visual cues that pilots used to estimate their altitude during the flare.

However, University of Oklahoma pilots most frequently used the horizon or end of runway, whereas Oklahoma State University pilots used the shape of runway or runway markings.

Softer throttle reduction is helpful for a better flare performance. Not immediately IDLE. 

The force of the engines and of gravity are driving the airplane down and forward. When you rotate the nose up for the flare, some of the energy that was used to propel the airplane forward is now used to further arrest the force of gravity. You have less force available for forward motion, your speed necessarily decreases.

Flare energy

The rotation-to-flare requires energy to arrest downward momentum; the amount of energy required depends on the glide path angle, any differences in aircraft speed from target speed, and any acceleration/deceleration. It will be to your advantage to make the angle and speed differences the same for every landing.

The rotation-to-flare may or may not bleed airspeed, depending on aircraft flight idle and ground effect characteristics. Here are three examples:

 B747 — The combination of a very large wing span induced ground effect perfectly compensates for energy needed to arrest the descent. Once the descent has been arrested, the flight idle and ground effect result in no airspeed loss at all under most conditions. The airplane has to be flown onto the runway, it will not run out of speed and sit itself down.

GV — The GV also has a very large wing span and high flight idle engines. The descent can be arrested with very little loss of speed, but the airplane does lose speed gradually if held inches off the runway. But, once again, it should be flown onto the runway to avoid a long landing.

G450 — The G450 will lose about 5 knots in the rotation to flare, which is precisely the minimum speed increment to VREF. Once the rotation to flare is made, speed decay continues as flight idle and ground effect are not enough to maintain speed. Any exaggerated flare for the sake of touchdown will result in a significant loss of speed. Once again, the airplane should be flown onto the runway.




VERY SOFT LANDING

The FLARE curve it has two breaking points: The first, it is on (Xf, Hf) [Glide Slope breaking point] and the second one it is on Xtd flare breaking point [Flare breaking point it is at the touchdown point]. Ideally, from this point on the flare curve must be tangent to runway surface to make a soft landing.

Very soft landing, it is that the Pilot Flying does not let the green curve flare segment [beyond Xtd point] to descend below X axis.
That green curve segment after the touchdown point it can NOT descend below the X axis
It does tangent on the runway surface plane for smoothing landing.

When to flare

What "the book" says:

"Very few manufacturers specify a flare height. Gulfstream, for example, leaves you off at 50 feet and the next thing you know, you are in the touchdown zone. Some Bombardier manuals say, "at or below 50 feet." About the only manufacturer that does print a height is Boeing. In their Boeing 777 Flight Crew Training Manual, they say this: "Initiate the flare when the main gear is approximately 20 to 30 feet above the runway by increasing pitch attitude approximately 2° - 3°. This slows the rate of descent." That pretty much agrees with what we did in the Boeing 747." 

You should consult your manufacturer's books.

HINT

Pilot after crossing over threshold at the height 50 ft for landing MUST NOT LET his/her eye corners to capture any lateral vision, mainly Pilot Flying, because of lateral vision illusion which induces pilot to see a relative movement speeding up the plane. That false relative speed up produces a sensation of runway length it would be shortening very quickly. Runway excursion.






segunda-feira, 8 de julho de 2013

Asiana Flight 214 - Much Slower Than Target Speed for Approach and Landing


UPDATE JULY 8, 2013 18:45 (UTC) 15:45 Brazilian Time

 
 
 
 
SAN FRANCISCO/SEOUL, South Korea  — The pilot of the crashed Asiana plane at San Francisco airport was still "in training" for the Boeing 777 when he attempted to land the aircraft under supervision, the South Korean airline said Monday.
 
São Francisco/Seul, Coreia do Sul – O piloto do avião da Asiana acidentado no aeroporto de  São Francisco estava ainda “em treinamento” para o Boeing 777 quando ele tentava pousar a aeronave sob supervisão, a empresa aérea South Korea disse nesta Segunda-feira (08 JUL).
Lee Kang-kook, the second most junior pilot of four on board the Asiana Airlines aircraft, had 43 hours of experience flying the long-range jet when it crashed Saturday, Asiana said.
 
Le Kang-kook, o segundo piloto mais junior dos quatro a bordo da aeronave da Asiana Linhas Aéreas, tinha 43 horas de experiência voando o jato de longo alcance quando ele acidendou no Sábado (06 JUL), disse a Asiana.
It was his first attempt to land a 777 at San Francisco, though he had flown there 29 times previously on different types of aircraft, said South Korean transport ministry official Choi Seung-youn.
Era a primeira tentativa de pousar um [Boeing 777] em São Francisco, apesar que ele tinha voado para lá 29 vezes perviamente em diferentes tipos de aeronave, disse o ministro dos transportes da Coreia do Sul, Choi Seung-youn.
Earlier, the ministry said Lee Kang-kook had accumulated a total of 9,793 flying hours, including his 43 at the controls of the 777.
Anteriormente, o ministro disse que Lee Kang-kook tinha acumulado um total de 9793 horas, incluindo 43 delas nos controles do [Boeing] 777.
 

Investigação: pilotos tentaram abortar o pouso logo antes do jato acidentar

by Jaxon Van Derbeken and Demian Bulwa
Updated 20:40, Sunday, July 7, 2013


Click on the link below to watch the first preliminary NTSB's report about Asiana Flight 214



Human Translation
by George Rock


(07-07) 20:33 PDT SAN FRANCISCO -- The doomed Asiana Airlines jetliner had its throttles set to idle and was moving so slowly that it nearly stalled before it smashed into  seawall bordering a San Francisco International Airport runway, federal investigators said Sunday.

(07 JUL) 20:23 O jato condenado da Asiana teve suas alavancas de potência ajustadas para Marcha Lenta e estava voando  tão lentamente que ele quase estolou (perdeu sustentação em voo) antes dele colidir contra um dique em volta da pista do aeroporto internacional de San Francisco, investigadores federais disseram Domingo (07).

The crew tried to abort the landing and avert the disaster, which killed two teenagers on board and injured dozens more, but it was too late, according to a preliminary review of flight data and cockpit communications by the National Transportation Safety Board.

 

A tripulação tentou abortar o pouso e evitar o desastre, o qual matou duas adolescentes a bordo e feriu dúzias a mais, mas era muito tarde, de acordo com  uma revisão preliminar pela NTSB sobre os dados do voo e comunicações no cockpit.

 

The crew sought to accelerate 7 1/2 seconds before impact, investigators said. Three seconds later, a vibrating "shaker stick" in the cockpit signaled an impending stall - a condition in which the wings lose lift and a plane can't be controlled.

A tripulação buscou acelerar 7.5 segundos antes do impacto, disseram investigadores. Três segundos depois, uma vibração do “stick shaker”(coluna do manche vibra]  no cockpit sinalizou  um estol iminente – uma condição na qual as asas perdem sustentação e um avião não pode ser controlado. [em baixíssima altitude].

And with 1 1/2 seconds left, someone on board alerted an air traffic controller that the Boeing 777 jetliner would try to pull up and circle around. It could not, and at 11:27 a.m. it bounced and skidded across the ground, losing its tail before it came to rest on the side of Runway 28L.

E com 1.5 segundos restante, alguém a bordo alertou um controlador de tráfego aéreo que o jato Boeing 777 tentaria arremeter e circular em volta. Ele não pôde, e às 11:27 A. M. Ele quicou e deslizou no solo, perdendo sua cauda antes dele parar na lateral da pista 28 da Esquerda.

National Transportation Safety Board Chairwoman Deborah Hersman said at a media briefing Sunday that it was too early to say what caused the crash of Asiana Flight 214, which was completing an 11-hour trip from Seoul with 307 people aboard.

A presidente da NTSB Deborah Hersman disse numa entrevista com a midia nesse Domingo que era muito cedo para dizer o que causou o acidente do Asiana Voo 214, o qual estava completando um viagem de 11 horas de Seul com 307 pessoas a bordo.

"Everything is on the table right now," she said

“ Tudo está na mesa neste momento”, ela disse.

Much slower than target.

Muito mais lento do que o alvo

However, Hersman focused her comments on the speed of the plane, which she said was "significantly below" the target speed of 137 knots, or 158 mph. "We're not talking about a few knots here or there," she said.

Todavia, Hersman focou os comentários dela na velocidade do avião, a qual ela disse estar “significante abaixo”da velocidade alvo de 137 Knots, ou 253 Km/h. “Nós não estamos falando de [defasagem] de poucos Knots aqui ou ali”,  ela disse.

"The approach proceeds normally as they descend. There is no discussion of any aircraft anomalies or concerns with the approach," Hersman said. When the throttles were finally advanced seconds before impact, she said, "the engines appear to respond normally."

 

“A aproximação prosseguia normalmente enquanto eles desciam. Não há discussão de qualquer anomalia na aeronave ou preocupações com a aproximação”,  disse Hersman. Quando as alavancas de potência foram finalmente avançadas segundos antes do impacto, ela disse, “os motores pareceram responder normalmente”.

At that point, though, the crew may have been powerless to regain altitude, said aviation experts. They were surprised that the jetliner's throttles were apparently idling at such a low altitude, which would have caused a lag time when they were finally pushed.

 

Nesse ponto, apesar desse fato, a tripulação pode ter estado sem potência para reganhar altitude, disse um especialista em aviação. Eles foram surpreendidos de que as alavancas de potência do jato estavam aparentemente na posição Marcha-Lenta em tal altitude baixa, o que teria causado um  intervalo de tempo  quando elas foram finalmente empurradas. [aceleradas].

"If you do that prior to impact, you're not going to have enough time to advance the throttles," said Barry Schiff, a former pilot for TWA who has written extensively about aviation safety. "You should always make an approach with power, and they didn't do that."

 

“Se você faz isso antes do impacto, você não está indo ter tempo suficiente para avançar as alavancas de potência”, disse Barry Schiff, um ex-piloto da TWA que tem escrito extensivamente acerca de segurança de aviação. “Você devia sempre fazer um aproximação com  potência, e eles não fizeram  isso”.

He added, "These pilots have a lot of explaining to do."

Ele acrescentou, “Estes pilotos têm uma porção de explicação a fazer”.

Jim Tilmon, a former commercial pilot and aviation consultant in Arizona, said the plane appeared to be "behind the power curve" - meaning that when the crew tried to throttle up the Pratt & Whitney engines, they may not have had enough altitude to arrest their descent.

 

Jim Tilmon, um ex-piloto comercial e um consultor de aviação no Arizona, disse que o avião pareceu estar  “atrás da curva de potência” – significando que quando a tripulação tentou acelerar os motores Pratt & Whitney, eles não puderam ter tido altitude suficiente para interromper a descida.

"It sounds like, too low, too slow, too late," Tilmon said.

“Isso soa como, muito baixo, muito baixo muito tarde”,  disse Tilmon

Hersman said one part of the investigation would look at whether the crew of the jetliner was affected by the loss of a key piece of airport equipment, known as a glide slope indicator, that had not been operating since June 1 because of a runway expansion project.

Hersman disse que uma parte da investigação olharia se a tripulação do jato foi afetada pela perda de uma peça chave do equipamento do aeroporto, conhecida como um indicador de rampa de planeio, que não tinha estado operando desde o dia 01 Junho por causa de um projeto de expansão da pista.

The system - designed for weather conditions that decrease visibility - gives pilots a 3-degree descent path to the runway. A needle on the instrument panel moves up and down and tells the pilot whether the plane is coming in too high or too low.

 

O sistema – planejado para condições meteorológicas que diminuem a visibiidade – dá aos pilotos uma trajetória de descida com 3 graus de ângulo para a pista. Uma agulha no painel de instrumentos move-se para cima e para baixo e diz ao piloto se o avião está voando muito acima da rampa de planeio ou muito abaixo.

Good visibility

Boa visibilidade

Hersman warned against drawing any conclusions about the loss of the indicator. Other systems were available to guide the plane, she said, including color-coded lights that show red when planes approach too low. It was a clear day with good visibility, she said, and the pilots were cleared for a visual approach.

 

Hersman alertou contra esboçar quaisquer conclusões acerca do indicador. Outros sistemas estavam disponíveis para guiar o avião, ela disse, incluindo luzes de cores codificadas que se mostram vermelhas quando aviões aproximam-se muito baixo. Estava um dia limpo com boa visibilidade, ela disse, e os pilotos estavam autorizados para uma aproximação visual.

Aviation experts said the loss of the glide slope system should not have been a problem. However, they added that, if it was working, it might have warned the Asiana Airlines crew of the impending danger.

Especialistas em aviação disseram que a falta do sistema da Rampa de Planeio não devia ter sido um problema. Todavia, eles acrescentaram quem se ele estivesse funcionando, ele poderia ter avisado à tripulação da Asiana Linhas Aéreas do perigo iminente.

"Let's face it, pilots were making approaches to that runway all day long," Schiff said. "You don't need to have that when conditions are clear. You can see the runway ahead of you."

 

“Vamos encarar isso, pilotos estavam fazendo aproximações para aquela pista durante todo o dia”,  disse Schiff. “Você não precisa ter isso quando as condições [meteorológicas] são boas. Você pode ver a pista em frente de você”.

Data collected by FlightAware, which tracks navigation broadcasts for sale to the aviation industry, showed the plane was dropping fast. When the plane was 600 feet over the bay, the company said, its descent was nearly twice as fast as normal.

 

Dados coletados pela Flight Aware, a qual rastrea navegação e transmite comercialmente para a indústria de aviação, mostrou que o avião estava descendo rápido. Quando o avião estava a 600 pés sobre a baia, a empresa disse, sua descida estava perto de duas vezes tão rápida quanto a normal.

The investigation, which will take months, will be aided not only by the recovery of the flight data recorder, but also by the survival of the pilots, said Kevin Hiatt, a former chief pilot for Delta Airlines who heads the Flight Safety Foundation in Alexandria, Va.

A investigação, a qual levará meses, será auxiliada não somente para recuperação do gravador de dados de voo, mas também  pela sobrevivência dos pilotos, disse Kevin Hiatt, um ex-piloto chefe da Delta Airlines que chefia a Flight Safety Foundation em Alexandria, Virginia.

"Investigators are going to be able to talk to them about whether there were any other factors that got them into that situation," Hiatt said.

“Investigadores vão ser capazes de conversar com eles acerca se houve quaisquer outros fatores que os fizeram entrar nessa situação”,  disse Hiatt.